As if someone were to buy several copies of the morning paper to assure himself that what it said was true.
Excerpt from: Algorithms to Live By: The Computer Science of Human Decisions by Brian Christian and Tom Griffiths
As if someone were to buy several copies of the morning paper to assure himself that what it said was true.
Excerpt from: Algorithms to Live By: The Computer Science of Human Decisions by Brian Christian and Tom Griffiths
But Marshall… not only admits his tricks… he seems to revel in them. On one episode of his [then] top-rated Laverne and Shirley series, for example, he says, “We had a situation where Squiggy’s in a rush to get out of his apartment and meet some girls upstairs. He says: ‘Will you hurry up before I lose my lust?’ But in the script we put something even stronger, knowing the censors would cut it. They did; so we asked innocently, well, how about ‘lose my lust’? ‘That’s good,’ they said. Sometimes you gotta go at ’em backward.”
On the Happy Days series, the biggest censorship fight was over the word “virgin.” That time, says Marshall, “I knew we’d have trouble, so we put the word in seven times, hoping they’d cut six and keep one. It worked. We used the same pattern again with the word ‘pregnant.’”
Excerpt from: Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion by Robert Cialdini
The French social scientist Claudia Fritz has examined, in various settings, the preferences of accomplished violinists for instruments made by old Italian masters like Stradivari. Everyone knows, if only from hearing of these incredibly valuable instruments being left in the backs of taxicabs, how lush and resonant they must sound, as if bestowed with some ancient, now lost magic. Who would not want to play one? But the expert musicians she has tested tend to prefer, under blind conditions, the sound of new violins.
In his book Strangers to Ourselves, Timothy Wilson has argued that we are often unaware why we respond to things the way we do; much of this behavior occurs in what he calls the “adaptive unconscious.”
Excerpt from: You May Also Like: Taste in an Age of Endless Choice by Tom Vanderbilt
Another frightening example comes from the realm of medicine. This time participants were given information on the effectiveness of treatments as a percentage of those cured overall (ranging from 90 to 30 percent). This is known as base rate information. They were also given a story, which could be positive, negative, or ambiguous.
For instance, the positive story read as follows: Pat’s decision to undergo Tamoxol resulted in a positive outcome. The entire worm was destroyed. Doctors were confident the disease would not resume its course. At one-month post-treatment, Pat’s recovery was certain.
The negative story read: Pat’s decision to undergo Tamoxol resulted in a poor outcome. The worm was not completely destroyed. The disease resumed its course. At 1-month post-treatment, Pat was blind and had lost the ability to walk.
Subjects were then asked would they undergo the treatment if they were diagnosed with the disease. Of course, people should have relied upon the base rate information of the effectiveness of treatment as it represented a foil sample of experience. But did this actually happen?
Of course not. Instead the base rate information was essentially ignored in favor of the anecdotal story. For instance, when participants were given a positive story and were told the treatment was 90 percent effective, 88 percent of people thought they would go with the treatment. However, when the participants were given a negative story and again told the treatment was 90 percent effective, only 39 percent of people opted to pursue this line of treatment.
Conversely, when told the treatment was only 30 percent effective and given a negative story, only 7 percent said they would follow this treatment. However, when low effectiveness was combined with a good story, 78 percent of people said they would take the drug. As you can see, the evidence on effectiveness of the treatments was completely ignored in favor of the power of the story.
Excerpt from: The Little Book of Behavioral Investing: How not to be your own worst enemy by James Montier
The reason for Tolkien’s mistake, since you ask, is that adjectives in English absolutely have to be in this order: opinion-size-age-shape-colour-origin-material-purpose Noun. So you can have a lovely little old rectangular green French silver whittling knife. But if you mess with that word order in the slightest you’ll sound like a maniac. It’s an odd thing that every English speaker uses that list, but almost none of us could write it out. And as size comes before colour, green great dragons can’t exist.
Excerpt from: The Elements of Eloquence: How to Turn the Perfect English Phrase by Mark Forsyth
When experiencing heightened emotions, people often mistakenly attribute the cause of arousal to the wrong source. The mind does not make clear and accurate assessments of why we feel a certain mood.
In a famous experiment, young men were asked to cross a high, dangerous suspension bridge. Whilst on the bridge, they interacted with a young female experimenter who offered them the opportunity to call her afterwards to ‘further discuss the research’. The group who met the woman on a high, dangerous bridge showed a much higher propensity to call the woman afterwards vs the control group who met the same woman on a safe bridge. Men in the dangerous bridge condition mistook their high state of emotional arousal for romantic attraction.
Excerpt from: The Unseen Mind by Ogilvy Change
Decision by committee needs to be scrapped. Group decisions are becoming more and more common in business, but when it comes to advertising, the result is often a very costly and public mess.
When the consensus of a large number of people has to be reached, the most likely outcome is predictable and safe work. “They sit there in committees day after day, and they each put in a color and it comes out grey.”
Allan Sherman, American writer and television producer.
Excerpt from: How To Make Better Advertising And Advertising Better by Vic Polinghorne and Andy Palmer
What keeps the relationship honest, trusting and mutually beneficial is nothing other than the prospect of repetition.
In game theory, this prospect of repetition is known variously as ‘continuation probability’ or ‘w’. Robert Axelrod has poetically referred to it as ‘the shadow of the future’. It is agreed by both game theorists and evolutionary biologists that the prospects for cooperation are far greater when there is a high expectation of repetition than in single shot games. Clay Shirky has even described social capital as ‘the shadow of the future at a societal scale’. Yet businesses barely consider this at all (in fact procurement, by setting shorter and shorter contract periods, may be unwittingly working to reduce cooperation).
Yet there are, when you think about it, two different approaches to business. There is the ‘tourist restaurant’ approach, where you try to make as much money from people on their single visit. And then there is the ‘local pub’ approach, where you make less money from people on each visit, but you profit(?) more over time by encouraging people to come back. The second type business is much more likely to generate that + yield positive sum outcomes then the first.
Excerpt from: Eat Your Greens by Wiemer Snijders
Particularly amusing is this recent telephone survey: a company wanted to find out, on average, how many phones (landline and cell) each household owned. When the results were tallied, the firm was amazed that not a single household claimed to have no phone. What a masterpiece.
Excerpt from: The Art of Thinking Clearly by Rolf Dobelli
In one study of simulated driving led by David Strayer and colleagues at the University of Utah, subjects talking on their phones “missed seeing up to 50 percent of their driving environments, including pedestrians and red lights.” (They were also ten times more likely to not stop at a stop sign.) Another experiment by Strayer and colleagues found that people talking on their phones had slower reaction times than drivers with a blood alcohol level at the legal limit.
What causes these mental deficits? The scientists blame inattention blindness, which occurs whenever the amount of information streaming into the brain exceeds our ability to process it.
Excerpt from: The Smarter Screen: Surprising Ways to Influence and Improve Online Behavior by Shlomo Benartzi and Jonah Lehrer
The idea for Velcro, conceived by George de Mestral, occurred whilst out walking his dog. Burdock seeds were always getting caught in his dog’s fur as she ran through the fields. De Mestral, an engineer, inspected further, and found the seeds hooked onto the fur with a series of microscopic loops. And so Velcro was born. It would go on to be used extensively, from children’s trainers to boots for the moon landing. Good dog.
Excerpt from: Brutal Simplicity of Thought: How It Changed the World by M&c Saatchi
Irony itself can be elusive to define, but in ads it usually means the ridiculing of conventional persuasive techniques. As far back as 1932, Jack Benny told this joke about the sponsor of his radio show: ‘I was driving across the Sahara Desert when I came across a party of people who had been stranded for 30 days without a drop of water, and they were ready to perish. I gave each of them a glass of Canada Dry Ginger Ale, and not one of them said it was a bad drink.’
Excerpt from: 100 Ideas That Changed Advertising by Simon Veksner
Just because something’s new doesn’t make it better. And just because you can do something, it doesn’t necessarily mean you should. We all know the advertising industry is obsessed with the word ‘new’, not just as a selling mechanism, but also a descriptor of its own corporate structures.
How many times have you read in advertising journals of the launch of a new agency with a new way of working? The advertising business is obsessed with the word ‘new’. Of course, ‘a new way of working’ with technology represents an embracing of evolving technologies and their opportunities, but sometimes in advertising we can behave like a child at Christmas who just keeps opening one present after another and never stops to play. It’s a case of: give me something new. New is good, old is bad. We talk about old technology as though it were bad and new as though it were virtuous. We need to have the wisdom to stand back and consider the gifts we’ve been given and how best to employ them.
Excerpt from: Hegarty on Advertising: Turning Intelligence into Magic by John Hegarty
Experiences are not remembered equally, our memories are encoded with the experiences (both positive and negative) at their peak ‘most intense’ point and their ending ‘concluding moment’.
Participants experienced both of the following conditions:
Hand submerged in 14°C ice water for 30 seconds.
Hand submerged in 14°C ice water for 30 seconds followed by an additional 30 seconds while the water heated up to 15°C.
When asked which trial they wished to repeat, subjects actually counter-intuitively opted for the second, longer condition.
That is, exactly the same amount of time in the colder water, only to end a little warmer.
Excerpt from: The Unseen Mind by Ogilvy Change
One day Ralph Waldo Emerson and his son tried to get a calf into the barn. But they made the common mistake of thinking only of what they wanted: Emerson pushed and his son pulled. But the calf was doing just what they were doing: he was thinking only of what he wanted; so he stiffened his legs and stubbornly refused to leave the pasture. The Irish housemaid saw their predicament. She couldn’t write essays and books; but, on this occasion at least, she had more horse sense, or calf sense, than Emerson had. She thought of what the calf wanted; so she put her maternal finger in the calf’s mouth and let the calf suck her finger as she gently led him into the barn.
Excerpt from: How to Win Friends and Influence People by Dale Carnegie
In a provocative exploration of this idea, nursery school children were asked to draw a picture with what was then a novel drawing tool: felt-tip markers. Some were offered a prize for drawing a picture with the markers; others received the prize unexpectedly, after they had already drawn their picture; still others were offered no incentive at all. When the markers were later introduced into a free play period, the children who had drawn a picture in order to get a reward played with them significantly less often that children who had not been “bribed” to draw their picture. In essence, the promise of a reward turned play into work. But when the prize was unexpected – when it was experienced not as a bribe but as a bonus – it did not decrease the children’s interest in playing with the markers.
Women had been fighting a long battle for respite from labour pains, and the survey made it plain that the battle was yet to be won. For decades, there had been widespread opposition to pain relief in labour, because it was deemed to go against the word of God. (‘In sorrow thou shalt bring forth children,’ the sinful Eve was told – Genesis 3:16.) But two events started to turn things around. One was the discovery that chloroform had anaesthetic properties. The other was that Queen Victoria secretly called a doctor to the birth of her eighth child, Prince Leopold, in 1853 and demanded that he give her some of this new-fangled chloroform to get her through. The palace denied the event for several years, but it nevertheless helped to disseminate the idea that taking pain relief in labour was an acceptable thing to do.
Excerpt from: The Life Project: The Extraordinary Story of Our Ordinary Lives by Helen Pearson
In 1972 the American meteorologist Edward Lorenz wrote a paper with an arresting title: “Predictability: Does the Flap of a Butterfly’s Wings in Brazil Set Off a Tornado in Texas?” A decade earlier, Lorenz had discovered by accident that tiny data entry variations in computer simulations of weather patterns—like replacing 0.506127 with 0.506—could produce dramatically different longterm forecasts. It was an insight that would inspire “chaos theory”: in nonlinear systems like the atmosphere, even small changes in initial conditions can mushroom to enormous proportions. So, in principle, a lone butterfly in Brazil could flap its wings and set off a tornado in Texas even though swarms of other Brazilian butterflies could flap frantically their whole lives and never cause a noticeable gust a few miles away. Of course Lorenz didn’t mean that the butterfly “causes” the tornado in the same sense that I cause a wineglass to break when I hit it with a hammer.
Excerpt from: Superforecasting: The Art and Science of Prediction
In 1985, at the dawn of the computer age, the psychologist Susan Belmore conducted a simple experiment on twenty undergraduates at the University of Kentucky. The students were exposed to eight different short texts and then asked to answer a series of questions about what they’d just read. Four of the passages appeared on paper (a sheet of white bond, single-spaced, forty-seven characters per line) and four appeared on the monitor of an Apple II Plus 48k computer. Belmore was curious if reading the text on a screen might influence both the speed of reading and levels of comprehension.
The results were depressing, at least if you were an early adopter of computer technology. “These data indicate that reading texts on a computer display is not equivalent to reading the same texts on paper,” Belmore wrote. “Overall, college students took 12 percent longer to read and comprehended 47 percent less with computer-presented text.”
Excerpt from: The Smarter Screen: Surprising Ways to Influence and Improve Online Behavior by Shlomo Benartzi and Jonah Lehrer
Sometimes, increasing a statement’s truthiness can be as simple as adding an irrelevant picture. In one rather macabre experiment from 2012, Newman showed her participants statements about a series of famous figures – such as a sentence claiming that the indie singer Nick Cave was dead. When the statement was accompanied by a stock photo of the singer, they were more likely to believe that the statement was true, compared to the participants who saw only the plain text.
The photo of Nick Cave could, of course, have been taken at any point in his life. It makes no sense that someone would use it as evidence – it just shows you that he’s a musician in a random band,’ Newman told me. ‘But from a psychological perspective it made sense. Anything that would make it easy to picture or easy to imagine something should sway someone’s judgement.’
Excerpt from: The Intelligence Trap: Why Smart People Do Stupid Things and how to Make Wiser Decisions by David Robson
Max Planck, the theoretical physicist who helped lay the groundwork for quantum theory, said: “A new scientific truth does not triumph by convincing its opponents and making them see the light, but rather because its opponents eventually die, and a new generation grows up that is familiar with it.”
Excerpt from: Hit Makers: The Science of Popularity in an Age of Distraction by Derek Thompson
Amazingly, just the opposite is true for propaganda. If it strikes a chord with someone, this influence will only increase over time. Why? Psychologist Carl Hovland, who led the study for the war department, named this phenomenon the sleeper effect. To date, the best explanation is that, in our memories, the source of the argument fades faster than the argument. In other words, your brain quickly forgets where the information came from (e.g. from the department of propaganda). Meanwhile, the message itself (i.e., war is necessary and noble) fades only slowly or even endures. Therefore, any knowledge that stems from an untrustworthy source gains credibility over time. The discrediting force melts away faster than the message does.
Excerpt from: The Art of Thinking Clearly by Rolf Dobelli
A key principle here is ‘the generation effect’ – that is, the finding that a message is significantly better remembered if the audience actually thinks it themselves, rather than just reading it superficially. Researchers at the University of Toronto assigned participants to one of two conditions: half of them read pairs of words that were associated in some way, such as rhyming or being semantically linked, like rapid-fast; while the other half were shown one word and the initial letter of its pair, like rapid-f_____. Afterwards, participants completed a test of recognition for the matched words. Those who simply read the words scored an average of 69%, while those who mentally generated the words scored 85%.
Excerpt from: Hooked: Revealing the hidden tricks of memorable marketing by Patrick Fagan
Researchers have gone to a great deal of trouble to test the efficacy of group brainstorming. In a typical experiment, participants arrive in a group. Half of them are randomly chosen to be in the ‘work as a group’ condition and are placed in one room. They are given standard brainstorming rules and have to come up with ideas to help solve a specific problem (perhaps design a new ad campaign, or find ways of easing traffic congestion). The other half of the participants are asked to sit alone in separate rooms, are given exactly the same instructions and tasks and asked to generate ideas on their own. Researchers then tally the quantity of ideas produced under the different conditions, and then experts rate their quality. So do such studies show that group brainstorming is more effective than individuals working alone? Many scientists are far from convinced. Brian Mullen from the University of Kent at Canterbury and his colleagues analysed the efficacy of group brainstorming in this way, and were amazed to discover i the vast majority of experiments, the participants working on their own produced a higher quantity and quality of ideas than those working in groups.
Excerpt from: 59 Seconds: Think a little, change a lot by Richard Wiseman
As John Ward of England’s B&B Dorland noted, “Advertising is a craft executed by people who aspire to be artists, but is assessed by those who aspire to be scientists. I cannot imagine any human relationship more perfectly designed to produce total mayhem.”
Excerpt from: Hey, Whipple, Squeeze This: The Classic Guide to Creating Great Ads by Luke Sullivan and Sam Bennett
My former partner Rich Silverstein used to talk about effective advertising using the analogy of those dot-to-dot games we all used to play as children. I’m sure you remember joining numbered dot to numbered dot. trying to guess what you’re drawing as the picture slowly emerges. Dot, to dot, to dot… then, with just one stroke of the pencil, it is suddenly clear. You have a picture of a badger. Silverstein always used to say that it was important for us to join enough of the dots in our advertising to avoid confusion (and as a result rejection), but to leave enough dots for the viewers or listeners to join for themselves. Into the gaps between the dots of advertising they should insert their own experience, hopes, fears, joys, and sorrows, and thus embrace the communication by becoming a part of it.
Excerpt from: Perfect Pitch: The Art of Selling Ideas and Winning New Business by Jon Steel
Einstein was a great fan of this technique. He said that: “To stimulate creativity, one muse develops the childlike inclination for play.” Researchers at the North Dakota State University agree. They conducted an experiment where they asked 76 undergraduates what they would do if college were cancelled for the day. The interesting bit was that half of them were encouraged to think as if they were seven years old. These students were found to give much more creative responses than the control group.
Excerpt from: Go Luck Yourself: 40 ways to stack the odds in your brand’s favour by Andy Nairn
When looking to the future, we tend to overestimate the good stuff and underestimate the bad.
This is a draft chapter from my new book; Security Gems: Using Behavioural Economics to Improve Cybersecurity (working title).
Subscribe to read new chapters as I write them.
Marriage. It’s a wonderful thing, isn’t it?
In the Western world, the numbers don’t agree. Divorce rates are about 40 percent.
That means that out of five married couples, two will end up in divorce. But when you ask newlyweds about their own likelihood of divorce, they estimate it at zero percent.
Good luck to them!
Optimism bias is sometimes used interchangeably with ‘overconfidence’, and refers to the phenomenon whereby individuals believe they are less likely than others to experience a negative event.
As humans we need some level of optimism, if we went in to marriage thinking it would end in divorce, marriage simply would not exists.
The optimism bias is an intriguing concept that comes with a host of benefits, such as shielding us from depression and ensuring we respond positively to failure.
Sadly, though, the optimism bias in cyber security leaves us overly-vulnerable to cyber attack.
When I was growing up, there was a kid in my neighbourhood who loved climbing trees. I was always suspicious one of his parents was a monkey.
He’d shoot up them, without a second thought.
Once, thirty metres in the air, a branch broke beneath him. All of us standing below heard the crack. It sounded like lightning, followed by a heavy thud as it hit the ground
Luckily he managed to quickly reach out and grab a branch above, saving himself from a long fall.
Whilst the slip didn’t bring him back down to earth, it did bring him back to reality. It took him the rest of the day to climb back down. And weeks before we saw him up another tree.
The dangers of being overly optimistic or self-confident can often blind us to the very high likelihood of negative outcomes.
When there’s nothing to warn us of our impending doom we get even more reckless.
Drink and drug driving is a massive problem, and is in a large part a result of our unbounding optimism.
“I’ve only had a couple of beers”, offers no solace to the family whose love one has been killed as a result of impaired reaction times.
Nightclubs in Germany came up with a brilliant idea to reduce the problem of their patrons jumping into cars after a night on the tiles; piss screens.
Urinals allowed drivers to steer a car in a video games using their pee. Aim left to go left. Right to go right.
If you’re too slow or swerve too much, that is to pee on the blokes foot next to them, the car would crash. “Too pissed to drive”, the screen would read, along with the number of the local taxi firm.
Again, in life we need moments to peg us back to reality.
When people receive emails they don’t necessarily treat them with the suspicion they deserve.
Far too often, we’re optimistic about the outcome of clicking links, and end up clicking malicious links or opening malicious attachments.
Wether it’s drink driving, or clicking an email. Both can have catastrophic consequences.
Facebook do a great job of warning us about the result of our actions. Click an external link on your newsfeed and they’ll make you confirm the link shown is where you want to end up.
The aim here is to make the negative effects and losses of a certain action clear to the individual, and offer a clear, safer alternative.
Sadly Facebook don’t do this with uploading drunk photos yet.
Now, I’m not advocating we all become pessimists. World economies rely on optimism.
Entrepreneurs need optimism.
Do you ever find yourself in situations wondering “how hard could it be?”.
As an amateur home-chef, I have a particularly bad habit of asking this type of question when dining out. How hard could it be to create a menu? Cook the food? Leave the customers wanting more?
I make a great Pad Thai.
In my town one particular restaurant unit has changed hands five times in as many years. Italian. Indian. Thai. Greek. Italian, again.
It’s not unusual. In some cities, the chance of restaurant failure in the first year can be as high as 90%. That is, nine out of every ten restaurants opened will fail!
Nine in ten! Who would want to open a new restaurant?
Restaurateurs know the numbers, but despite the well-documented failure rates, they often don’t think they apply to them. They might argue their concept is different to the others, their restaurant is in a better part of town, or the cuisine is seeing new popularity.
But do they really have a better chance of success than others trying the same thing?
In the majority of cases, no.
The problem is we don’t know the reason behind the facts. We don’t know a lot about others, but know a lot about ourselves.
We’re optimistic about ourselves, we’re optimistic about our kids, we’re optimistic about our families, but we’re not so optimistic about the guy sitting next to us, and we’re pessimistic about the fate of our fellow citizens and the fate of our country.
This plagues those responsible for creating public health messaging.
One in two UK people will be diagnosed with cancer in their lifetime. But despite the odds most people don’t think they’ll get cancer [1].
38 percent of cancer cases are preventable in the UK. 15 percent of that can be attributed to stopping smoking.
Yet millions of people still smoke, pouring their hard earned money into the pursuit of lowering their health outcomes.
People explain it away. They go to the gym everyday. Other smokers don’t. They don’t drink, like other smokers.
Comparative optimism, where we can’t make a direct comparison, convinces us others are more likely to suffer negative experiences than we are ourselves.
Studies around peoples perceived privacy risks, like unauthorised access to accounts and sharing of personal information, is much more likely to happen to other people [2].
Almost half of all UK businesses suffered some form of cyber security breach in 2020 [3].
Yet companies don’t think it will happen to them.
It’s why we can ignore network security risks while at the same time reading about other companies that have been breached. It’s why we think we can get by where others failed.
Optimism induced invincibility needs to be accounted for, and removed. You are no better than your peers, mostly.
Skiing. Windsurfing. Rock climbing. These are the kinds of things I love to do on holiday.
Health insurance companies don’t like me doing them. I know this because they charge me a hefty premium for coverage.
Previously I was guilty of questioning if travel insurance was worth the money.
Whilst speaking to the Swiss Mountain Rescue team one Winter, they told me just how much it cost to be evacuated via helicopter. About $100 per minute. And that’s from takeoff to landing.
Perceptions of actual risk can be clouded by optimism. I don’t go on holiday to break a leg, but the chance is pretty high.
It’s not just that we don’t think bad things can happen to us or are more likely to happen to someone else. We–all things being equal–believe that good outcomes are more probable than bad outcomes.
In one study, participants were given a list of 18 positive and 24 negative events, like getting a good job after graduation, developing a drinking problem, and so on [4].
Overall, they considered themselves 15% more likely than others to experience positive events, and 20% less likely than others to experience negative events.
People are more likely to accept risks if they feel they have some control over them.
Here we see the feeling of security diverging from the reality of security.
Controlling for this feeling is important.
We all know someone that has “seen it all”.
Experience often trumps decision making. It offers a sense of security.
But never let it cloud the actual risks, which should be assessed with an eye of experience, but also an eye of fatalism.
You are not invincible.
[1] Cancer risk statistics
[2] Optimistic bias about online privacy risks
[3] Almost half of UK businesses suffered a cyber attack in past year
[4] Unrealistic Optimism about Future Life Events
This post is a draft chapter from my new book. Pardon the typos.
We seek out or interpret information that confirms our preconceptions, avoiding things that challenge them.
This is a draft chapter from my new book; Security Gems: Using Behavioural Economics to Improve Cybersecurity (working title).
Subscribe to read new chapters as I write them.
According to the flat Earth model of the universe, the sun and the moon are the same size.
You’ll find credible looking mathematical models that argue the theory. Photographs taken from a plane showing a flat horizon. Queries about how the seas could ever exist if the earth was round.
You won’t find calculations from Eratosthenes who is credited for discovering the earth was round. Photographs taken from space of a round planet. Or mentions of gravity, which holds the water in the seas.
Or does it?
As humans we have a disposition to confirm our beliefs by exclusively searching for information that supports a hunch while excluding opposing data.
Confirmation bias isn’t limited to conspiracy theorists. It causes us to vote for politicians, investors to make poor decisions, businesses to focus on the wrong ideas, and almost certainly led you to buy this book.
During the 2008 US presidential election, Valdis Krebs analysed purchasing trends on Amazon. People who already supported Obama were the same people buying books which painted him in a positive light. People who already disliked Obama were the ones buying books painting him in a negative light. [1]
People weren’t buying books for the information. They were buying them for the confirmation.
I’m in no doubt the people buying this book have a predisposition for product psychology.
Sound like you?
I love the word “yes”.
Yes, have an extra slice of cake. Yes, you do look good today. Yes, you are the best.
Experiment after experiment has shown that people tend to ask questions that are designed to yield a “yes”.
This is also known as congruence heuristic [2].
Google search histories are a good demonstration of the affirmative questions we all love to ask.
“Are cats better than dogs?”
We prime Google that cats are indeed better than dogs. Google hears we have a preference for cats. Google plays ball, listing sites detailing reasons why cats are better than dogs.
“Are dogs better than cats?”
The same question phrased differently produces entirely different results. Now dogs are better.
“Which is better; cats or dogs?”
Or;
“What is the best pet for [my situation]?”.
Would have been better questions. Obviously the answer is always dogs.
Affirmative approaches to reasoning are common in security.
Analysts enter an investigation digging for an answer they really want. They are worried about their manager pulling them up because they’ve not found anything juicy. The CISO needs their shiny dashboard showing number of threats detected.
Teams lose sight of the bigger picture.
Such an approach creates blindspots because people are looking for what they know, instead of considering other possibilities, the negative test cases.
I hate the word “No”.
No, you can’t have an extra slice of cake. No, you don’t look good today. No, you are not the best.
It’s hard to accept something that conflicts with what we believe. So-much-so our brains have developed a coping mechanism of sorts.
Imagine you’ve spent years of research into a particular area of study.
Late nights in the lab trying to uncover evidence to support you hypothesis. Weekends spent fretting over calculations. Months lost scouring obscure libraries.
All to prove the world is flat.
So much knowledge makes it easy to explain away a “no”.
A picture of earth from space.
That’s Hollywood magic at work.
Tides.
Well, “Isaac Newton is said to have considered the tides to be the least satisfactory part of his theory of gravitation”. “Duh!”. [3]
People tend to not change their beliefs on complex issues even after being provided with research because of the way they interpret the evidence.
Capital punishment is another polarising issue, but one that also draws on our moral compass.
In one experiment, a mix of participants who were either in support of, or against capital punishment were shown the same two studies on the subject.
After reading the detailed descriptions of the studies, participants still held their initial beliefs and supported their reasoning by providing “confirming” evidence from the studies and rejecting any contradictory evidence, or considering it inferior to the “confirming” evidence. [4]
We can all be guilty of trying to explain aways why things that don’t conform to what we believe.
“Well, that could never happen. Our firewall will block that type of thing”.
And we’re a stubborn bunch.
I’ve had some silly arguments in my time. Backing down in the heat of an argument with a partner can be hard at the time, but laughable an hour later.
Politics is a similarly laughable pursuit.
Many people hold an allegiance to the same political party their whole life.
Democrats questioned why people still voted Republican when Trump was on the card, despite of all the evidence questioning the reality of his claims to “Make America Great Again”.
Evidence might hold a strong position in the court of law. In the court of public opinions it’s not so strong.
In fact, not only is it not so strong, it can work against our reasoning! People’s preexisting beliefs are not only explained away when challenged by contradictory evidence, they have been shown to actually get stronger! [5]
All is not lost though.
Whilst one piece of disconfirming evidence does not result in a change in people’s views, it has been shown a constant flow of credible refutations can correct misinformation and misconceptions.
Think about how you disseminate your research.
Before forensic science became an integral part of the criminal justice system, eyewitness accounts were the basis of a prosecutor’s case.
The problem is our memory just isn’t particularly good. We remember some things and forget others. It tries to link memories together for easier recall, often falling victim to confirmation bias, amongst others in the process.
“Was the car speeding or not speeding, ma’am?”.
“Yes, officer. I heard the engine revving loudly.”
Confirmation bias influences eyewitnesses to make non-factual assumptions.
A revving engine might be linked to speeding in one mind. A mechanic might recognise this as a badly tuned engine, completely unrelated to speed.
Hundreds of wrongful convictions have been overturned in recent years as a result of cases bought solely on eyewitness accounts for this very reason.
The future is strongly influenced by memories of experiences in our past. It’s a fundamental to becoming the best.
Which is great if you’re trying to perfect a free kick into the top corner, but often falls short in many other areas. Like reading the resumes of job applicants.
Oxford University; advance to interview. Likes cats; nope.
In one scenario, individuals were asked to read a woman’s profile detailing her extroverted and introverted skills. Half were asked to assess her for either a job of a librarian or salesperson.
Those assessing her as a salesperson better recalled extroverted traits while the other group recalled more examples of introversion [6]. Their memories told them the best sales were extroverted and vice-versa.
Before long your team talks the same, thinks the same, and dresses the same. They thrive of validating their same outlook on the world.
To quote Eminem; “Would the Real Slim Shady please stand up?”.
Management consultants love to hark on about the benefits of seeing things from a different perspective. And they’re right.
Sometimes a breath of fresh air can give you a new take on security strategy.
Try to prove yourself wrong.
[1] New Political Patterns
[2] Heuristics and Biases in Diagnostic Reasoning (Baron, 2000)
[3] Earth Not a Globe
[4] Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence (Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979)
[5] The Backfire Effect
[6] Testing hypotheses about other people: The use of historical knowledge (Snyder, M., & Cantor, N.,1979)
This post is a draft chapter from my new book. Pardon the typos.
We remember things that stand out in the crowd. But different doesn’t necessarily mean it’s important.
This is a draft chapter from my new book; Security Gems: Using Behavioural Economics to Improve Cybersecurity (working title).
Subscribe to read new chapters as I write them.
To “stand out like a sore thumb” implies that something is noticed because it is very different from the things around it.
I’m often guilty of being the sore thumb. Dressed in shorts mid-winter, whilst those around are being warmed by five layers of clothing.
One of the factors behinds EasyJet’s success, arguably the pioneer of the low-cost flight, was to stick out like a sore thumb. The companies early advertising consisted of little more than the airline’s telephone booking number painted in bright orange on the side of its aircraft.
“Have you heard of that orange airline?”, people would ask.
Have you ever highlighted information in a book? Then you too have used this effect to your advantage.
Psychologists have studied why our attention is usually captured by salient, novel, surprising, or distinctive stimuli. Probably using highlighter during their research.
Product designers understand our fascination with things that stand out and will spend hours perfecting the size, colour and shape of something to grab your attention, directing you on the path they want you to take.
Good products guide users to the important features and functions by making them stand out.
The big red flashing bell indicating a security alert should be distinctive, drawing attention and making it very clear that it needs to be looked at.
Being able to draw attention to something in the age of information overload is vital.
An email received from a friend or family member sticks out amid a sea of unfamiliar names.
A letter where the address is handwritten stands out, allowing me to easily filter boring correspondence from correspondence I will enjoy reading.
“YOU’VE WON A PRIZE”
“YOUR ACCOUNT HAS BEEN COMPROMISED”
These email subject lines have a similar effect.
Not only is someone shouting at you, they’re also warning you of a potentially serious event that arouses a sense of urgency.
It’s not your everyday (or hourly); “Sally has liked your photos taken in 2003 on Facebook” email. It’s serious.
In phishing school [2], you’ll find classes titled: How to grab a victims attention.
Successfully grabbing the attention of someone browsing their inbox is the first part of a successful campaign. You should expect the attackers to have aced that class.
Digging deeper into the email inbox, or not as the case may be, it’s clear our brains weren’t designed to deal with mountains of spam.
So called alert fatigue highlights this weakness. People stop noticing alerts, emails, texts, and [INSERT LATEST COOL MESSAGING SERVICE HERE] because there are simply too many.
People become desensitised to similar things being shown to them every day.
I once sat with a client who somewhat proudly proclaimed the “Alerts” folder in his inbox stood at 10,000 unread emails. That was nothing he assured me, his colleagues folder clocked closer to six digits!
You don’t want to foster this culture.
When my fire alarm sounds, my heart rate accelerates as adrenaline is pumped into my blood stream. The noise that stands out. It’s important. It immediately draws all my attention. Yes, even from an oh so cute cat video.
Security alerting needs to have the same effect. To point you to real fires. To prioritise what is most important. Missing critical alerts, emails, texts, or warnings of actual fires does not typically end well.
The ability to recognise and remember things that stand out has long proved advantageous to our species.
As hunter gatherers being able to determine something that stood out was vital in finding food and avoiding becoming food.
Evolution has long realised standing out is a disadvantage.
Chameleons.
The Artic Hare is another great example of the evolutionary importance of blending in.
In the winter their bright white coats hide them from predators amongst a backdrop of snow. In spring, the hare’s colours change to blue-gray in approximation of local rocks and vegetation.
Humans are no different.
Go to a club on a Saturday night and watch the herds of men and women dressed head to toe in clubbing uniforms.
During my college years flannel shirts were the “in-thing”. One night I bumped into 3 other guys, who all had a great taste in fashion I will add, all wearing the same shirt.
Militaries around the world understand the importance of camouflage. Soldiers don’t want to stand out. It’s a matter of life and death on the battlefield.
Neither do criminals.
Actors know downloading terabytes of data in a short period of time will stand out. Instead they slowly exfiltrate data over months patterns don’t stand out.
Malware is designed to act like a user, disguising itself as a normal process on an endpoint.
Yet so much of cyber security is focused on identifying the anomalies.
Sure, anomalies are important. It’s why so many vendors consistently demo that there product proudly detected “3 failed logons, from 3 different locations, in 3 seconds, for 1 account”.
However, the things that stick out, in a world where the bad guys are doing everything they can to stay anonymous, are only part of the story.
In the early days of map making it took a lot of time to produce a map.
Companies had to hire someone to go out and walk every street.
Needless to say, plagiarism plagued the pre-computerised map making industry.
In the 1930’s, General Drafting, a map making company, came up with an ingenious idea. In their map of New York State they included a copyright trap; a fictitious place, Agloe [3].
Fast forward a few years and the company spotted Agloe detailed on a map produced by one of their fiercest competitors, Rand McNally.
Such was the problem, Agloe continued to appear on a number of maps up until the 1990s. I can imagine the disappointed faces of day-trippers, and the ensuing arguments about wrong turns.
These traps have come to be affectionately known as Mountweazels [4]: a bogus entry deliberately inserted in a reference work. Prizes for anyone who spots the one in this book.
Like Mountweazels, honeypots are similar traps used in computer networks.
A honeypot mimics a system that may be attractive to an attacker, but would only ever be accessed by someone snooping around.
Like a motion activated light illuminates intruders attracted by the shiny objects in your house, honeypots illuminates attackers attracted by the shiny potential they offer.
If you want people to remember something, make it stand out.
[1] Salience, Attention, and Attribution: Top of the Head Phenomena (Taylor & Fiske, 1978)
[2] Completely fictitious.
[3] Agloe, New York (Wikipedia)
[4] Fictitious entry (Wikipedia)
This post is a draft chapter from my new book. Pardon the typos.